Saturday, March 31, 2007

Some Book Reviews

Blink. Think. Blank. Bunk

How Doctors Think;

In recent years, Groopman writes, there has been a sharp reaction against the “catch as catch can” approach to teaching diagnosis that prevailed when he was in medical school, where trainees would watch senior doctors and somehow absorb their way of thinking. But he is critical of much of the thinking now in vogue. Today’s physicians are increasingly encouraged to behave as if they were computers, and to reason from flowcharts and algorithms. This is intended to produce better diagnoses and fewer errors; it is also embraced by insurance companies, who use it to decide which tests and treatments to approve. This approach can be useful for “run-of-the-mill diagnosis and treatment — distinguishing strep throat from viral pharyngitis, for example,” Groopman writes. But for difficult cases he finds it limiting and dehumanizing. He is similarly critical of generic profiles, classification schemes that draw statistical portraits of disease states. They encourage the doctor to focus on the disease, not the patient, and so may lead him to miss the particular manifestation in the particular sufferer.

Groopman also discusses physician heuristics — shortcuts to decision-making that he considers “the foundation of all mature medical thinking,” although they too “can lead to grave errors.” But Groopman points out that heuristics aren’t taught in medical school and are in fact discouraged, in favor of a much more leisurely and extended kind of thinking, typified by Socratic dialogue between students and professors. This means that young doctors enter the hospital knowing little about either the advantages or the disadvantages of heuristics. Take, for example, what psychologists call “availability,” the tendency to judge the likelihood of an event by how readily it comes to mind. Thus physicians may mistake symptoms of one disease for those of another disease they’ve seen more often. Or they may fall prey to “confirmation bias,” which leads them to rapidly assemble information into an accurate diagnosis — or misconstrue the evidence before them.

Cognitive errors are highlighted in Groopman’s account of a Vietnamese infant he calls Shira Stein, brought home by her adoptive mother to Boston. Before departure, the baby was coughing. On landing, she seemed dehydrated and refused to drink; a day later she was in the I.C.U. at Children’s Hospital with severe pneumonia. Her doctors, recognizing they might be up against an exotic infection from Vietnam, struggled to oxygenate her, but even on the respirator her condition deteriorated. Saved from death by extreme measures, she was found to be infected by five potentially lethal agents: pneumocystis, cytomegalovirus, Klebsiella, Candida and parainfluenzae. Such devastating multiple infections implied an immune deficiency. Doctors concluded that Shira had SCID, severe combined immunodeficiency disorder.

SCID is a rare, inherited condition. Because the gene is located on the X chromosome, it’s especially rare in girls, who must inherit the gene from both parents. And because Shira’s T-cells were only slightly low, she didn’t show the usual SCID pattern. Her case was defined as “atypical.”

Groopman’s account focuses on Shira’s extraordinary mother, who week after week stayed in the hospital, steadfast in her belief that her daughter would recover. “Shira is going to live,” Rachel tells him. “I can feel it inside of me.”

After more than a month in the I.C.U., Shira was successfully weaned from the respirator, and plans were begun for a bone marrow transplant to treat her immune deficiency. With the child improving, Rachel conducted her own research on SCID, becoming ever more convinced that her daughter did not have it. Instead, she suspected Shira had a nutritional deficiency.

Rachel stubbornly insisted that immune testing be repeated, and the pediatricians reluctantly indulged her. Shira’s T-cells were normal; she did not have SCID after all. The bone marrow transplant may well have killed her.

Groopman reviews the clinical conference where Shira’s case was discussed. Such conferences occur at every teaching hospital in the country, Groopman writes, but they generally lack “an in-depth examination of why the diagnosis was missed — specifically, what cognitive errors occurred and how they could have been remedied.” He observes that the doctors at Boston Children’s Hospital, one of the best pediatric hospitals in the world, had extensive experience with SCID and similar genetic abnormalities: “Familiarity breeds conclusions and sometimes a certain degree of contempt for alternatives.” Physicians may be reluctant to pursue unlikely diagnoses, particularly if they will be criticized by colleagues for ordering too many tests or for being show-offs.

No comments: